Thursday, 31 December 2015
Saturday, 26 December 2015
Sleep isn’t needed to create long-term memories – just time out
Need to remember something important? Take a
break. A proper one – no TV or flicking through your phone messages. It seems
that resting in a quiet room for 10 minutes without stimulation can boost our
ability to remember new information.
The effect is particularly strong in people
with amnesia, suggesting that they may not have lost the ability to form new
memories after all.
“A lot
of people think the brain is a muscle that needs to be continually stimulated,
but perhaps that’s not the best way,” says Michaela Dewar at
Heriot-Watt University in Edinburgh, UK.
Memory boost
New
memories are fragile. They need to be consolidated before being committed to long-term
storage, a process thought to happen while we sleep. But at least some consolidation may occur while we’re
awake, says Dewar – all you need is a timeout.
In
2012, Dewar’s team showed that having a rest helps a person to remember what
they were told a few minutes earlier. And the effect seems to last. People who
had a 10-minute rest after hearing a story remembered 10 per cent more of it a week
later than those who played a spot-the-difference
game immediately afterwards.
“We dim the lights and ask them to sit in an
empty, quiet room, with no mobile phones,” says Dewar. When asked what they had
been thinking about afterwards, most volunteers said they had let their minds
wander.
Now
Dewar, along with Michael
Craig at the University of Edinburgh and their
colleagues, have found that spatial memories can also be consolidated when we
rest.
Volunteers who rested after exploring a
virtual-reality environment were 10 per cent more accurate at orientating
themselves in relation to virtual landmarks than those who played a game of
spot the difference afterwards.
It’s good to rest
These findings together suggest that simply
resting while we’re awake can give us some of the memory benefits thought to be
confined to sleep.
This
is good news for insomniacs. “As long as you’re reasonably relaxed, you might
still be experiencing some of the memory-consolidation processes that sleep
would normally do,” says Gareth
Gaskell at the University of York in the UK.
The research could have bigger implications
for people with amnesia. When Dewar’s team conducted a memory experiment with
people who had the condition, they saw more striking results. “Most of them
can’t lead a normal life because they can’t remember what they did 10 minutes ago,”
she says – but all showed huge improvements on the memory test when given a
break.
The volunteers were able to recall between 30 and 80 per
cent of a list of words when they rested for 9 minutes. Without a break, eight of the 12 were unable to
remember anything.
“The findings challenge current theories of
memory,” says Dewar. “It is typically assumed that people with amnesia lose the
ability to consolidate memories; they can take in information but it is rapidly
gone.”
Information overload
Dewar thinks that overstimulation may be what
causes memory problems in people with amnesia. “If we try to reduce the amount
of information going in, people with amnesia can form new memories. There is
some spare capacity there that we can tap into,” she says.
“It is very surprising and exciting,” says
Gaskell. “If we can understand how this takes place, we could help people with
amnesia,” he says.
Dewar hopes to investigate whether having
plenty of breaks can help people with amnesia to learn new information, such as
family news or how to navigate a new home. She has reason to be optimistic: the
wife of a man with Alzheimer’s who took part in her study says she has used the
technique to teach her husband the name of his new grandchild.
Monday, 14 December 2015
To push through goo, use itty, bitty propellers :
A
really nice example of bioinspired engineering, By stealing a trick
from bacteria, tiny human-made vehicles can cruise through goo.
Researchers
in Germany have designed metal
and glass micropropellers that travel through mucus in
part by liquefying their thick surroundings. The propellers mimic the
activity of ulcer-causing bacteria, and could help inform the design
of microbots or drug delivery systems.
Saturday, 12 December 2015
New Type of Carbon Is Harder and Brighter Than Diamonds
Scientists have designed a new type of carbon that is harder and
brighter than naturally formed diamonds.
For those who want to wear a one-of-a-kind sparkler on their
fingers, the new material, called Q-carbon, also gives off a soft glow.
"This new phase is very unique," said study co-author
Jagdish Narayan, a materials scientist at North Carolina State University.
"It has novel electrical, optical and magnetic properties."
For instance, the material can act as either a metal or a semiconductor, and is magnetic at room temperature.
Heat and pressure :- Despite being one of the
most ubiquitous and iconic symbols of wealth and luxury, scientists still don't
fully understand how diamonds are
formed. Most think the diamonds mined today formed between 1 billion and 3
billion years ago, at a depth of about 62 miles (100 kilometers) below the
Earth's surface, researchers
previously told Live Science.
Friday, 11 December 2015
New e-skin feels heat, textures and more :
It’s the first time anyone has demonstrated an e-skin that can sense so many different types of stimuli. It can relay signals directly to brain cells, based on tests with cells that had been isolated from a mouse. Those cells got the skin’s message, too. They became more or less active depending on how hard researchers pushed on the skin.
Such work offers a blueprint for scientists to actually “bridge electronics with biology.
One day, such an e-skin might cover prosthetic limbs and plug directly into people’s nerve cells, he says. This would let people know if they were touching something hot or rough or sharp — just as real skin does. The artificial skin also could form the basis for soft, wearable medical devices.
Over the last several years, scientists have invented an assortment of electronic-skin components. These range from different soft materials to new types of sensors. Some sensors can recognize more than one type of stimuli, but only under just the right conditions. E-skins are still “far from having the capabilities that human skin has,” he admits. But the newly reported advances do bring the prospect of such a technology closer.
By mimicking the ultrasensitive skin of human fingertips, Ko and his colleagues designed their e-skin to detect many types of signals. The researchers placed a soft ridged film over thin bumpy sheets made from plastic and Graphene. (Graphene is a single-atom-thick sheet of carbon with a host of unusual properties.) Those thin bumpy sheets are only about as thick of a few layers of the plastic cling-wrap used to seal up kitchen leftovers.
Touching this e-skin pressed together the electrodes on the bumpy sheets. This caused an electric current to flow through the device, which was hooked up to a machine to measure such signals. The amount of current depended on how much the bumps compressed. This provided a gauge of the pressure.
Heating the Korean team’s e-skin also generated a current, showing that it could sense temperature, too. A strip of the e-skin placed on a person’s wrist let the researchers simultaneously measure skin temperature and blood pressure.
And ridges on this simulated skin help it detect texture. When researchers skimmed its surface over glass or sandpaper, the ridges vibrated. The pattern of those vibrations differed depending on what had touched it. Sensors in the skin picked up on those differences.
Sound waves also made the e-skin vibrate. It could “hear” noise from a speaker playing a famous lecture by the late physicist, Richard Feynman. The e-skin converted his words into electrical signals, and then sent them to a machine. This let the researchers judge how well the e-skin sensed sounds. It worked even better than an iPhone’s microphone, Ko concluded.
Thursday, 10 December 2015
People are less willing to rely on their knowledge and say they know something when they have access to the Internet, suggesting that our connection to the web is affecting how we think.
Professor Evan F. Risko, of the
Department of Psychology at the University of Waterloo, led a recent study
where the team asked about 100 participants a series of general-knowledge
questions, such as naming the capital of France. Participants indicated if they
knew the answer or not. For half of the study, participants had access to the
Internet. They had to look up the answer when they responded that they did not
know the answer. In the other half of the study, participants did not have
access to the Internet.
The team found that the people who had
access to the web were about 5 per cent more likely to say that they did not
know the answer to the question. Furthermore, in some contexts, the people with
access to the Internet reported feeling as though they knew less compared to
the people without access.
"With the ubiquity of the
Internet, we are almost constantly connected to large amounts of information.
And when that data is within reach, people seem less likely to rely on their
own knowledge," said Professor Risko, Canada Research Chair in Embodied
and Embedded Cognition.
In interpreting the results, the
researchers speculated that access to the Internet might make it less
acceptable to say you know something but are incorrect. It is also possible
that participants were more likely to say they didn't know an answer when they
had access to the web because online searching offers an opportunity to confirm
their answer or resolve their curiosity, and the process of finding out is
rewarding.
"Our results suggest that access
to the Internet affects the decisions we make about what we know and don't
know," said Risko. "We hope this research contributes to our growing
understanding of how easy access to massive amounts of information can
influence our thinking and behaviour."
David McLean and Amanda Ferguson,
research assistants, are co-authors of the study, which appears in the journal, Consciousness
and Cognition. Professor Risko plans to further the research in
this area by investigating the factors that lead to individuals' reduced
willingness to respond when they have access to the web.
Wednesday, 9 December 2015
Scientists Spot A Planet That Looks Like 'Earth's Cousin'
T. Pyle/NASA/SETI Institute/JPL-Caltech
Scientists who have been hunting for another Earth beyond our solar system have come across a planet that's remarkably similar to our world.
It's almost the same size as Earth, and it orbits in its star's "Goldilocks zone" — where temperatures are not too hot, not too cold, and maybe just right for life.
But a lot about this planet is going to remain a mystery, because it's 500 light-years away.
Researchers detected the planet while poring over data collected by NASA's Kepler Space Telescope. The telescope spent years staring at 150,000 stars, watching for telltale dips in brightness that might mean a planet was circling around a star.
One small star in the direction of the constellation Cygnus showed signs of five planets. Four of them are tucked in close to the star, so they're probably too hot for life.
But the fifth planet looked special.
"This planet orbits its star every 130 days," says Elisa Quintana of the SETI Institute and NASA's Ames Research Center. It's called Kepler-186f, and it's just 10 percent bigger than Earth.
Kepler-22b, seen in this artist's rendering, is a planet a bit larger than Earth that orbits in the habitable zone of its star. Some researchers think there might be "superhabitable" worlds that may not resemble Earth.
THE TWO-WAY
In Search For Habitable Planets, Why Stop At 'Earth-Like'?
At least in our solar system, Earth-sized planets are made of rock and iron and gas, says Quintana, "so we can guess that Kepler-186f, being so close in size to Earth, has a high probability of being rocky also and composed of those sorts of materials."
This artist rendering provided by NASA, shows Kepler-11, a sunlike star around which six planets orbit.
THE TWO-WAY
'Planet Bonanza' Indeed: NASA Unveils 715 New Worlds
Conditions on the surface would depend on what kind of atmosphere it had, if any. If it was like Earth, temperatures wouldn't be balmy, Quintana says.
"Being on this planet would probably be like being in San Francisco on a cool day," she says. "It would be a much colder place to live."
It would be warm enough, however, for one thing that's thought to be essential for life. "If this planet had the right atmospheric conditions, and if there were water on the surface, it would be likely in liquid form," says Quintana.
But if it has oceans, they would look different.
"It's not going to have a deep rich blue ocean, such as we have, because there's less blue light coming from the star," says Tom Barclay of NASA's Ames Research Center, another member of the team that describes the planet in the journal Science. "So the ocean would probably be a duller, grayer blue."
And because this planet orbits a dim, red dwarf star, he notes that midday on this planet wouldn't be bright — it would look more like an hour before sunset on Earth.
"It's very romantic to imagine there'd be places out there that look like Earth, and that's what we're trying to find — places that remind us of Earth," Barclay says.
Although Kepler-186f shares characteristics with Earth, "it's not an Earth twin," he notes. "It isn't around the same type of star. It's perhaps more of an Earth cousin."
Still, it's the first time anyone has found an Earth-size planet in the habitable zone of a distant star, Barclay adds. "This is a really profound discovery. It's a major milestone."
Other experts on planets beyond our solar system agree that this discovery is a big deal.
"This planet really is the same size as the Earth and the same temperature," says David Charbonneau of Harvard University. "Up until this point, planets satisfied one of those two, but we really didn't have one that was both those things together."
Both those things are key to life on Earth, Charbonneau says, but we'll probably never know if this new planet has life. "And the reason," he explains, "is that this star system is just too far away from us."
Even though this planet is too distant for follow-up work with other telescopes, it suggests similar worlds might be out there orbiting other red dwarf stars, which are very common.
If scientists could find another planet like this around a nearby star, he says, "we could really study the atmosphere and really figure out something about whether it truly is Earth-like and maybe whether it actually has life on the surface."
That's why Charbonneau and other scientists will keep searching for other Earth-like planets closer to home.
Tuesday, 8 December 2015
Roboticists learn to teach robots from babies
Babies learn about the world by
exploring how their bodies move in space, grabbing toys, pushing things off
tables and by watching and imitating what adults are doing.
But when roboticists want to teach a
robot how to do a task, they typically either write code or physically move a
robot's arm or body to show it how to perform an action.
Now a collaboration between University
of Washington developmental psychologists and computer scientists has
demonstrated that robots can "learn" much like kids -- by amassing
data through exploration, watching a human perform a task and determining how
best to carry out that task on its own.
"You can look at this as a first
step in building robots that can learn from humans in the same way that infants
learn from humans," said senior author Rajesh Rao, a UW professor of
computer science and engineering.
"If you want people who don't know
anything about computer programming to be able to teach a robot, the way to do
it is through demonstration -- showing the robot how to clean your dishes, fold
your clothes, or do household chores. But to achieve that goal, you need the
robot to be able to understand those actions and perform them on their
own."
Saturday, 5 December 2015
Blue-eyed humans have a single, common ancestor:
Variation in the colour of the eyes from brown to green can all be explained by the amount of melanin in the iris, but blue-eyed individuals only have a small degree of variation in the amount of melanin in their eyes.
What is the genetic mutation
"Originally, we all had brown eyes," said Professor Hans Eiberg(Department of Cellular and Molecular Medicine). "But a genetic mutation affecting the OCA2 gene in our chromosomes resulted in the creation of a "switch," which literally "turned off" the ability to produce brown eyes." The OCA2 gene codes for the so-called P protein, which is involved in the production of melanin, the pigment that gives colour to our hair, eyes and skin. The "switch," which is located in the gene adjacent to OCA2 does not, however, turn off the gene entirely, but rather limits its action to reducing the production of melanin in the iris -- effectively "diluting" brown eyes to blue. The switch's effect on OCA2 is very specific therefore. If the OCA2 gene had been completely destroyed or turned off, human beings would be without melanin in their hair, eyes or skin colour -- a condition known as albinism.
Limited genetic variation
Variation in the colour of the eyes from brown to green can all be explained by the amount of melanin in the iris, but blue-eyed individuals only have a small degree of variation in the amount of melanin in their eyes. "From this we can conclude that all blue-eyed individuals are linked to the same ancestor," says Professor Eiberg. "They have all inherited the same switch at exactly the same spot in their DNA." Brown-eyed individuals, by contrast, have considerable individual variation in the area of their DNA that controls melanin production.
Nature shuffles our genes
Thursday, 3 December 2015
Android v/s ios (Stability of Apps and the Operating System)
The Crittercism Mobile Experience Report
published in March 2014 ranked Android KitKat as more stable than iOS 7.1.
Other findings from the report include:
Android 2.3 Gingerbread has the highest total
crash rate, at 1.7%. Other versions of Android — Ice Cream Sandwich, Jelly
Bean, and KitKat — have a crash rate of 0.7%.
iOs 7.1 has a crash rate of 1.6%., and the rates
for iOS 7.0 and iOS 5 are 2.1% and 2.5% respectively.
Phone versions of both Android and iOS are more stable
than their tablet versions.
Crash rates for apps vary by category — games are
most likely to crash (4.4% crash rate) and e-commerce apps have the lowest
crash rate of 0.4%.
Wednesday, 2 December 2015
Android v/s ios (In Terms of Security and Privacy)
Security:
Android’s applications are isolated from the rest of the system’s
resources, unless a user specifically grants an application access to other
features. This makes the system less vulnerable to bugs, but developer
confusion means that many apps ask for unnecessary permissions. The most
widespread malware on Android is one where text messages are sent to premium
rate numbers without the knowledge of the user, and the sending of personal
information to unauthorized third parties. As it is the more popular smartphone
operating system, it is more likely to be the focus of attacks.
Malware writers are less likely to write apps for iOS, due to Apple's
review of all the apps and verification of the identity of app publishers.
However, if an iOS device is jailbroken and apps installed from outside Apple's
store, it can be vulnerable to attacks and malware.
Privacy:
When it comes to protecting users' private information, iOS wins. When
installing apps on Android, the user is presented with all the permissions that
the app is requesting. This is an all-or-nothing proposition. The user can
choose to accept the app's request for permissions or not install the app at
all. App developers take advantage of this "feature" and request a
lot of user information. For example, Pandora's mobile app on Android requests
permissions for your Google identity, contacts, calendar, photos, media, files
and even call information.
Pandora's app on iOS gets no such permissions. After it is installed and
opened by the user, an iOS app may request additional permissions like location
and access to Contacts. But the user can reject these permission requests. Even
after approving the permission requests, iOS users can quickly glance at which
apps have access to their Contacts and location data, and turn off access for
apps with which they no longer want to share this data.
In November 2014, Twitter announced that it is now tracking the list of
apps its users have installed on their phones. Both iOS and Android are
vulnerable — an app installed on either platform can get a list of other apps
installed on the same device.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)